Please use this identifier to cite or link to this item: http://cris.unibe.edu.do/handle/123456789/408
DC FieldValueLanguage
dc.contributor.authorSánchez-Vincitore, Laura V.-
dc.contributor.authorCubilla-Bonnetier, Daniel-
dc.contributor.authorMarte-Santana, Hugo-
dc.date.accessioned2023-10-10T16:23:19Z-
dc.date.available2023-10-10T16:23:19Z-
dc.date.issued2023-
dc.identifier.citationFrontiers in Aging Neuroscience, 15, 1212496; 2023-
dc.identifier.urihttp://cris.unibe.edu.do/handle/123456789/408-
dc.description.abstractCognitive decline usually begins after individuals reach maturity, which is more evident in late adulthood. Rapid and constant cognitive screenings allow early detection of cognitive decline and motivate individuals to participate in prevention interventions. Due to accelerated technological advances, cognitive screening and training are now available to the layperson using electronic devices connected to the internet. Large datasets generated by these platforms provide a unique opportunity to explore cognitive development throughout life and across multiple naturalistic environments. However, such data collection mechanisms must be validated. This study aimed to determine whether the data gathered by commercial visuospatial and phonological working memory tests (CogniFit Inc., San Francisco, USA) confirm the well-established argument that age predicts cognitive decline. Data from 3,212 participants (2,238 females) who were 45 years old or older were analyzed. A linear regression analysis explored the relationship between age and working memory while controlling for gender, sleep quality, and physical activity (variables that are known to affect working memory). We found that age negatively predicts working memory. Furthermore, there was an interaction between age and gender for visuospatial working memory, indicating that although male participants significantly outperformed females, the relationship between age and working memory differs for females and males. Our results suggest that the computerized assessment of visuospatial and phonological working memory is sensible enough to predict cognitive functions in aging. Suggestions for improving the sensitivity of self-reports are discussed. Further studies must explore the nature of gender effects on cognitive aging.-
dc.language.isoEnglish-
dc.publisherFrontiers Media S. A.-
dc.relation.ispartofFrontiers in Aging Neuroscience-
dc.rights.urihttps://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/-
dc.subjectCiencias de la Salud-
dc.titleCognitive decline monitoring through a web-based application-
dc.typeResearch Report-
dc.rights.licenseThis is an open-access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License (CC BY). The use, distribution or reproduction in other forums is permitted, provided the original author(s) and the copyright owner(s) are credited and that the original publication in this journal is cited, in accordance with accepted academic practice. No use, distribution or reproduction is permitted which does not comply with these terms.-
dc.identifier.doihttps://doi.org/10.3389/fnagi.2023.1212496-
dc.rights.holder© 2023 Sánchez-Vincitore, Cubilla-Bonnetier, Marte-Santana and Duñabeitia.-
dc.contributor.affiliationLaboratorio de Neurocognición y Psicofisiología (NEUROLAB)-
dc.contributor.affiliationLaboratorio de Neurocognición y Psicofisiología (NEUROLAB)-
dc.contributor.affiliationLaboratorio de Neurocognición y Psicofisiología (NEUROLAB)-
dc.relation.issn1663-4365-
dc.description.volume15-
dc.description.issue1212496-
dc.subject.keywordsAge-associated cognitive decline-
dc.subject.keywordsCognitive decline-
dc.subject.keywordsAging-
dc.subject.keywordsWorking memory-
dc.subject.keywordsPhysical activity-
dc.subject.keywordsSleep quality-
dc.subject.keywordsNaturalistic environment-
dc.contributor.authorsSánchez-Vincitore, Laura V.-
dc.contributor.authorsCubilla-Bonnetier, Daniel-
dc.contributor.authorsMarte-Santana, Hugo-
dc.contributor.authorsDuñabeitia, J. A.-
dc.typeofaccessOpen Access-
dc.contributor.affiliationinstitutionUniversidad Iberoamericana (UNIBE)-
dc.contributor.affiliationinstitutionUniversidad Iberoamericana (UNIBE)-
dc.contributor.affiliationinstitutionUniversidad Iberoamericana (UNIBE)-
dc.contributor.affiliationinstitutionUniversidad de Nebrija-
dc.contributor.affiliationcountryDominican Republic-
dc.contributor.affiliationcountryDominican Republic-
dc.contributor.affiliationcountryDominican Republic-
dc.contributor.affiliationcountrySpain-
item.openairetypeResearch Report-
item.cerifentitytypePublications-
item.grantfulltextopen-
item.fulltextCon texto completo -
item.openairecristypehttp://purl.org/coar/resource_type/c_18cf-
item.languageiso639-1English-
crisitem.author.deptLaboratorio de Neurocognición y Psicofisiología (NEUROLAB)-
crisitem.author.deptLaboratorio de Neurocognición y Psicofisiología (NEUROLAB)-
crisitem.author.deptLaboratorio de Neurocognición y Psicofisiología (NEUROLAB)-
crisitem.author.parentorgUniversidad Iberoamericana (UNIBE)-
crisitem.author.parentorgUniversidad Iberoamericana (UNIBE)-
crisitem.author.parentorgUniversidad Iberoamericana (UNIBE)-
Appears in Collections:Publicaciones del NEUROLAB-UNIBE
Publicaciones indexadas en Scopus / Web of Science
Files in This Item:
File Description SizeFormat
fnagi-15-1212496.pdfFull text [open access]18.08 MBAdobe PDFView/Open
Show simple item record Recommend this item

Google ScholarTM

Citations

Altmetric

Mentions

Dimensions

Citations


This item is licensed under a Creative Commons License Creative Commons